Journal Information
Visits
103
Vol. 18. Issue 3. (In progress)
(July - September 2025)
Review
Full text access
Evidence on the parameters of oculomotor skills and normative values: A systematic review
Visits
103
Mario Cantó-Cerdána,b,
, Carlos Javier Hernández-Rodríguezc,d, Antonio Martínez-Abada,b
a Department of Optometry, Vissum (Miranza Group), Alicante, Spain
b Grupo de Investigación en Optometría (GIOptom), University of Alicante, Alicante, Spain
c Department of Optometry, OKYO Ophthalmics, Alicante, Spain
d Department of Ophthalmology, Quirónsalud Medical Center, Elche, Spain
This item has received
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Figures (2)
Tables (3)
Table 1. Characteristics of the 38 studies included in the review.
Tables
Table 2. Most commonly measured aspects of oculomotor parameters in the articles reviewed.
Tables
Table 3. Quality rating of the 38 included studies in the systematic review (QUADAS-2 results) (☺: low risk; ☹: high risk; ?: unclear risk.).
Tables
Show moreShow less
Additional material (1)
Abstract
Purpose

To evaluate the current evidence on oculomotor measurement parameters and their normative values through a systematic review.

Methods

A search of primary studies was conducted using a search equation with free language. Original articles analyzing normal oculomotor function parameters in healthy populations of any age, studies that included a clearly differentiated healthy control group, and articles using any oculomotor measurement test were included. The QUADAS-2 tool was used to assess the risk of bias, applicability, and quality of the studies. The review was conducted independently by the authors and then pooled to determine the final inclusion.

Results

A total of 915 articles were identified, of which 750 were excluded after the first review of the title and abstract. In the second step, 133 out of 165 investigations were discarded. Ultimately, 32 articles from the initial search were included, along with 10 additional articles identified through a manual search. The findings revealed variations in how oculomotor skills are measured, including differences in stimuli, measurement distances, and parameters assessed. A high risk of bias was observed (≥50 % in the areas of “flow and timing”, “reference standard” and “patient selection”) along with poor applicability (≥50 % in all aspects).

Conclusions

There is no clear evidence on normative values for oculomotor skills, nor is there a consensus on the measurement methods, stimulus used, or working distance. Furthermore, there is no agreement on which aspects of oculomotor skills should be assessed. To enhance reliability and applicability, measurement criteria should be standardized, and normative values should be established.

Key words:
Oculomotor skills
Oculomotor function
Systematic review
Gaze processing
Saccadic
Full Text
Introduction

Oculomotor function refers to an individual’s innate ability to execute eye movements in a seamless, coordinated, and fluid manner, ensuring the maintenance of a clear, fused, and stable image on the central region of the retina. When an object is in motion, it is crucial to sustain this visual stability as the object moves. To achieve accurate oculomotor performance, three fundamental skills must be assessed: tracking movements, saccadic movements, and fixation movements. These skills should be well developed monocularly and must also be coordinated to ensure efficient oculomotor function.1

Oculomotor skills are currently being studied across various disciplines within the health sciences.2 Most research is focused on the neurological field, including schizophrenia,3 cognitive impairment,4 depression,5 biomarkers of neurodegenerative diseases,6 neurodevelopmental disorders,7 and even post-COVID conditions.8 Additionally, oculomotor skills play a crucial role in non-health-related fields, such as marketing9 or enabling individuals with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis to communicate using eye-tracking technology.10

In the field of optometry, oculomotor studies have grown over the past decade, largely due to advancements in eye-tracking technologies.11 Research has focused on various areas, including visual dysfunctions, strabismus, amblyopia, nystagmus, and visual impairment. Additionally, oculomotor studies have extended to vision-related fields such as refractive surgery, sports vision or driving vision.2,11,12

However, despite this exponential growth, concerns remain regarding the diagnostic methodology for oculomotor abnormalities. No clear range of normal values has been established for the parameters used to assess ocular motility, and no gold standard or reference test has been defined.13 Classical tests, such as the Developmental Eye Movement Test (DEM),14 the Northeastern State University College of Optometry (NSUCO) Oculomotor Test,15 and the King-Devick Test (K-D)16 have been used for many years. However, these tests have the disadvantage of being subjective for both the examiner and the patient. With the advent of new eye-tracking technologies, equipment has been developed to measure oculomotor parameters objectively. However, these systems vary in the parameters they assess, the psychophysics of measurement, the stimulus used, and the measurement distance.11–13

Research has been conducted to establish normative ranges for different subjective tests,14–16 as well as for some of the new technologies.17–20 However, there is still no scientific consensus regarding the psychophysics of measurement, the range of values, or the parameters to be assessed. This lack of standardization highlights the need for a unified approach to these aspects.

Therefore, the aim of this study is to evaluate the current evidence on oculomotor measurement parameters and their normative values through a systematic review.

Methods

The study has been registered in the PROSPERO database. A comprehensive search was conducted using a search strategy (Supplementary Material) in three databases: PubMed, Web of Science, and Scopus. The search included all age groups and imposed no time restrictions to ensure that no relevant articles were overlooked. After the initial search, a refined selection of articles was made based on the following criteria:

Inclusion criteria

  • Original descriptive articles analyzing normal oculomotor parameters (pursuit, saccadic movements, fixation, saccadic velocity, and reading saccades) in a healthy population of any age.

  • Originals comparative articles analyzing oculomotor parameters, whose design includes a well-defined control group not accomplishing inclusion criteria.

  • Articles utilizing a subjective test currently used in the clinical practice such as NSUCO, DEM, ADEM, or K-D.

  • Articles utilizing an objective test currently used in the clinical practice such as eye-tracker, or video-oculography.

Exclusion criteria

  • Case series, clinical case reports, and animal studies.

  • Studies involving populations with neurological, developmental, or ocular pathologies that lack a well-defined control group or include fewer than 20 subjects.

  • Methods employing stimuli different to standard geometrical shapes (dot, cross, square or similar) or related to language (numbers, letters).

  • Methods providing non quantitative results such as graphs or interpretations.

  • Samples where visual evaluations were performed diagnosing binocular anomalies, such as amblyopia and strabismus in its subjects.

The article selection process followed a sequential approach. First, titles and abstracts were reviewed to exclude irrelevant studies, and duplicates were removed. Second, full-text articles were examined, and only those that met the predefined inclusion criteria and addressed the research question were selected. Third, a manual search considering studies known by articles and reviewing references of included articles was conducted to identify additional references that might not have appeared in the initial search. Articles with control groups that did not clearly report results were excluded.

Finally, to assess the risk of bias, applicability, and quality of the studies, the QUADAS-2 tool was used. This tool is divided into domains that evaluate key aspects of each article, such as participant selection, index test, reference test, and the flow and timing of the study. Following the guidelines of the evaluation tool, the risk of bias was classified into three evidence level groups: studies with a low risk of bias, studies with an unclear risk of bias, and studies with a high risk of bias.

The article selection and the quality analysis using the QUADAS-2 procedure were performed blindly and independently by three investigators to ensure accurate classification of the included studies. In cases of discrepancies, a consensus was reached between the authors, with all three authors discussing and agreeing. (REF QUADAS-2 2011 Withing)

The initial literature search was conducted in February 2024, and the databases were reviewed again in September 2024 using the same methodology.

Results

The study selection process for this systematic review is illustrated in a flowchart in Fig. 1. Initially, 915 documents were retrieved during the search. After reviewing titles and abstracts and removing duplicates, 165 articles were selected for full-text evaluation. Of these, 133 were excluded for not meeting the inclusion criteria. Ultimately, 32 articles were included. Additionally, a manual search identified 6 more articles, bringing the total to 38 articles that were analyzed in this review.

Fig. 1.

Flow chart of eligible papers used in the meta-analysis (PRISMA statement). PRISMA, preferred reporting items for systematic reviews.

Study characteristics

Table 1 presents the characteristics of the 38 included studies, which span from 1980 to 2023. The studies included sample sizes ranging from 20 to 2075 subjects. Of these, 22 studies (57.9 %) were descriptive cross-sectional, 15 studies (39.5 %) were comparative cross-sectional, and 1 study (2.6 %) was pseudo-experimental.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the 38 studies included in the review.

Year  Author  Characteristics of the control group (n, ranged age)  Study design  Oculomotor skills  Measurments system  Stimulus  Optometric examination  Work Distance (cm) 
1980  Schalen21  20, age 22–70  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  Electro-oculography  Light Spot in computer  No  160 cm 
1984  Bergenius22  60, age 11–70  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  Electro-oculography  Light Spot in computer  No  70 cm 
1990  Garzia14  534 normal subjects aged 6–13  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC  DEM  Numbers on test card  No  33 cm 
1992  Maples15  1714 normal subjects aged 5–14  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  NSUCO  Sphere  No  Not described 
1992  Versino23  76 normal subjects aged 12–77  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC  Electro-oculography  Light Spot  No  Not described 
1993  Ross24  53 normal subjects aged 7–15  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  Eyetracker  Small target in computer  No  43 cm 
1997  Litman25  24 normal subjects  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC, SP  Eyetracker  Bright Square  No  43 cm 
1997  Ross26  25 normal subjects  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC, SP  Eyetracker  White disc in screen  No  120 cm 
1999  Campana27  65 normal subjects  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC  Eyetracker  Light Spot  No  90 cm 
2003Jimenez281056 normal subjects aged 6–12Cross-sectional, descriptiveSACDEM and Scheiman & Wick  Numbers on test card and fixing pointYes40 cm // 33 cm DEM
procedure 
2006  Rutsche18  358 normal subjects aged 0–6  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  Pupil reflex  Color lights  Yes  40 cm 
2007  Aring29  135 normal subjects aged 4–15  Cross-sectional, descriptive  FIX  Infrared light  Dot  Yes  53 cm 
2010van Tritch3028 normal subjects aged 15–35Cross-sectional, comparativeSAC, SPDouble magnetic  Red light laser spotNoNot described
induction method 
2011  Goepel31  31 normal subjects age 7–12  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC  Eyetracker  Cartoons in computer  No  51,8 cm 
2011  Kattoulas32  2075 normal subjects aged 18–25  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP, FIX  Eyetracker  Cross  No  Not described 
2011  Webber33  59 normal subjects aged 9–10  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC  Eyetracker and DEM  Numbers on test card  Yes  Not described 
2012  Shi34  20 normal subjects aged 6–12  Cross-sectional, comparative  FIX  Eyetracker  Light spot  Yes  200 cm 
2012  Boot35  213 normal subjects aged 0–12  Cross-sectional, descriptive  FIX  Eyetracker  White Dots  No  60 cm 
2015  Seferlis36  250 normal subjects aged 18–70  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP, FIX  Video-oculography  Light spot  Yes  170 cm 
2015  Doettl37  62 normal subjects: 4–44  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC, SP  Videonystagmography  Snellen chart  Yes  127 cm 
2016  Zalla38  20 normal subjects  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC  Eyetracker  Grid of 13 points  No  60 cm 
2016  Yang39  50 normal subjects aged 20–69  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  Video-oculography  Point  No  100 cm 
2017  Choi40  63 normal subjects: aged 19–80  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC, FIX  Eyetracker  Text  No  85 cm 
2017  Ferreira41  31 normal subjects aged 18–45  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC  Eyetracker  Cross  No  70 cm 
2018  Rizzo42  42 normal subjects aged 19–52  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC  Eyetracker and K-D  Numbers on test card  No  Not described 
2018  Wetzel43  75 normal subjects aged 21–53  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC  Eyetracker  Nine separated points  No  75 cm 
2019  Hoffmann44  40 normal subjects  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC  Eyetracker  Circle  No  Not described 
2019  Chehrehnegar45  59 normal subjects  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC  Eyetracker  Cross (Simon task)  No  60 cm 
2019  Wetzel46  75 normal subjects aged 18–65  Cross-sectional, comparative  SAC, SP  Eyetracker  Nine separated points  No  75 cm 
2020  Wertli47  25 normal subjects aged 10–11  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, FIX  Eyetracker  Text  Yes  Between 50 to 80 cm 
2020  Sinno19  120 normal subjects aged 5–17  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  Videonystagmography  White Square  No  120 cm 
2021  Hindmarsh48  196 subjects aged 7.3–8.9  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP, FIX  Eyetracker and DEM  Numbers on test card  Yes  60 cm 
2021  Kullmann49  466 normal subjects aged 18–45  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, SP  Eyetracker  Dot  No  Not described 
2021  Amato50  54 normal subjects aged 18–28  Pseudoexperimental  SAC  Eyetracker  Videogame  No  Not described 
2022  D’Addio51  34 normal subjects  Cross-sectional, comparative  FIX  Eyetracker  White disc in screen  No  65 cm 
2022  Eichler52  52 normal subjects aged 9–34  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC  Eyetracker  OMAT  Yes  25 cm 
2023  Orduna-Hospital53  52 normal subjects aged 18–30  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, FIX  Eyetracker and DEM  Numbers on test card  Yes  60 cm 
2023  Wertli54  118 normal subjects aged 7–12  Cross-sectional, descriptive  SAC, FIX  Eyetracker  Text  Yes  Between 50 to 80 cm 
  SAC: saccadicSP: smooth pursuitsFIX: fixationDEM: developmental eye movementOMAT: oculomotor assessment tool test 

Regarding oculomotor skills, 34 out of the 38 studies (89.5 %) addressed saccadic movements, 16 studies (42.1 %) focused on tracking movements, and 11 studies (28.9 %) investigated fixation movements. In terms of technology and measurement systems, a significant amount of variability was found, with classical tests such as the DEM test, NSUCO test, and electro-oculography in studies performed >30 years ago, alongside newer technologies, like eye trackers, video-oculography, and video-nystagmography in recent evidence. The stimuli used also varied widely, including letters, texts, reading tests, numbers, and monochromatic, colored, or illuminated Figs. It difficulted the stratified analysis by type test. Similar limitation was found for age because 18 of 38 studies (47.4 %) was performed exclusively in children but ones in babies and others in adolescents, 15 of 38 (39.5 %) were performed exclusively in adults, and 5 of 38 (13.2 %) was performed combining both population. Notably, only 12 of the 38 studies (31.6 %) conducted an optometric examination prior to the oculomotor assessment. Finally, regarding the working distance for measuring oculomotor values, variability was observed, ranging from 25 cm to 200 cm. It is important to note that 9 studies (23.7 %) did not specify the working distance, and 2 studies (5.3 %) provided a range but did not specify an exact value.

Oculomotor skills aspects

Table 2 presents the aspects of oculomotor skills measured for each parameter (saccadic, smooth pursuit, and fixation movements), extracted from the studies included in this systematic review. It is evident that there is considerable diversity in the parameters analyzed for each test. Additionally, it can be observed that most of these parameters are not analyzed in >20 % of the articles.

Table 2.

Most commonly measured aspects of oculomotor parameters in the articles reviewed.

Skills  Aspects  n  % 
SaccadesNumber of saccades  11  28.9 
Amplitude of saccades  21.1 
Velocity of saccades  21.1 
Latency of saccades  11  28.9 
Smooth pursuits  Pursuit gain  10  26.3 
Fixation  Fixation time  23.7 

For saccadic movements, the most frequently analyzed parameters are the number of saccades (28.9 %), latency (28.9 %), velocity (21.1 %) and amplitude (21.1 %). In pursuits movements, the most commonly measured aspect is pursuit gain (26.3 %). Lastly, for fixation movements, the most frequently measured parameter is fixation time (23.7 %). In addition, authors did not find reported data on Bivariate Contour Ellipse Area (BCEA), a standard metric of fixation stability.

Other aspects were also measured by the authors of the included articles, such as Peak Velocity in saccadic movements, velocity in smooth pursuits and number of fixations in fixation movements. However, these aspects were measured less frequently, in fewer than 20 % of the articles.

Assessment of quality and bias of included studies

Table 3 and Fig. 2 present the analysis of the 38 included studies using the QUADAS-2 tool, which evaluates the risk of bias and applicability. In general, it can be observed that there is a high risk of bias (≥50 % in the aspects of “flow and timing”, “reference standard” and “patient selection,” as well as poor applicability (≥50 %), across all aspects.

Table 3.

Quality rating of the 38 included studies in the systematic review (QUADAS-2 results) (☺: low risk; ☹: high risk; ?: unclear risk.).

Study  Risk of biasApplicability concerns
  Patient selection  Index test  Reference standard  Flow and timing  Patient selection  Index test  Reference standard 
1980 Schalen21  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
1984 Berginus22  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
1990 Garzia14  ☹  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
1992 Maples15  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
1992 Versino23  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
1993 Ross24  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☺ 
1997 Litman25  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
1997 Ross26  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☹ 
1999 Campana27  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2003 Jimenez28  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☺  ☺  ☺ 
2006 Rütsche18  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹ 
2007 Aring29  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☺  ☺ 
2010 van Tritch30  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2011 Goepel31  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2011 Kattoulas32  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☺ 
2011 Webber33  ☺  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☺  ☺  ☺ 
2012 Shi34  ☺  ☹  ☺  ☹ 
2012 Boot35  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☹ 
2015 Seferlis36  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☺  ☺ 
2015 Doettl37  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2016 Zalla38  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2016 Yang39  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
2017 Choi40  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2017 Ferreira41  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2018 Rizzo42  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2018 Wetzel43  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2019 Hoffman44  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2019 Chehrehnegar45  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2019 Wetzel46  ☹  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2020 Wertli47  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
2020 Sinno19  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☺  ☺ 
2021 Hindmarsh48  ☺  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☺  ☺ 
2021 Kullman49  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☺ 
2021 Amato50  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2022 D’Addio51  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹  ☹ 
2022 Eichler52  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺  ☺  ☺ 
2023 Orduna-Hospital53  ☺  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☺  ☺ 
2023 Wertli54  ☺  ☺  ☹  ☹  ☺ 
Fig. 2.

QUADAS-2 domain for articles included in the systematic review.

Discussion

Oculomotor function has gained increasing relevance in recent years. In fact, more than half of the studies analyzed in this review (20 out of 38; 52.6 %) have been conducted in the last decade.19,36–54

Firstly, the technology used to measure oculomotor function has advanced considerably. Early studies relied on electrooculography and subjective assessments, whereas more recent research utilizes eye-tracking systems and video-nystagmography (Table 1). This shift suggests a notable improvement in the quantitative analysis of ocular motor skills. Secondly, it is now demonstrated that oculomotor disturbances may be present in some systemic conditions, such as neurological diseases3-6,50 or development disorders.7 Despite current devices seeming to measure oculomotor function in a better way, it is not completely demonstrated because there is a great methodological variability between studies in terms of the type of stimulus, the complexity of the task, the working distance, and even the analyzed parameters. Such variability precludes direct comparison of measurements across different systems and constrains the ability to perform agreement analyses between devices. Consequently, there are very few studies comparing subjective methods (DEM, K-D, NSUCO) with objective methods based on eyetrackers,33,42,48 and even fewer comparing several objective methods with one another. Not only are comparisons between devices not adequately developed in scientific literature, but also the validation with a single device is also insufficient because typically only one measurement is taken for each subject, preventing the development of repeatability analysis. In fact, only the DEM14 and NSUCO15 techniques have been thoroughly validated by measuring twice for each subject and calculating intrasession repeatability. The impossibility of generalizing the results is also supported by the study design and population. Oculomotor function is highly age-dependent,14,15,18,19,32,36,55 and there are multiple studies involving adults, children or both. In contrast, almost half of the studies analyzed in this paper (15 of 38 studies; 39.5 %)25–27,30,34,37,38,40–46,51 are cross-sectional with comparative design incorporating a control group whose ocular movements are compared to those obtained by a specific group with a particular condition or disorder. This involves selecting the control group based on the study group’s characteristics, inducing a patient selection bias that prevents the results from being generalized to the broader population. Another finding observed in this systematic review, potentially affecting the inference process, is the lack of an optometric evaluation in the majority of studies (26 of 38 studies; 68.4 %).14,15,19,21–27,30–32,35,38–46,49–51 Among 12 articles that conducted previous eye tests,18,28,29,33,34,36,37,47,48,52–54 only one study performed a comprehensive optometric examination evaluating accommodation, vergences, and stereopsis.53 Eight articles conducted some accommodative and binocular tests, but these were insufficient for the purposes of this study,18,28,29,33,47,48,52,54 and the other three articles only included VA measures.34,36,37 It represents a very remarkable limitation of the current evidence because an evaluation of refraction, accommodation and vergences would be elementary in this topic as indicated Cacho-Martínez et al. (2024 Cacho-Martínez) Moreover, difficulty in focusing or maintaining single (haplopic) vision is related to the efficiency of fixations and saccades. Therefore, a prior optometric examination is important to detect binocular or accommodative dysfunctions that may affect oculomotor performance (REF 2022, Liu Z)

It is reported that oculomotor function is impaired in individuals with amblyopia56 and strabismus,57 affecting both the dominant and amblyopic eye.58 Additionally, monocular and binocular oculomotor skills differ.59 In cases of convergence insufficiency, alterations in reading saccades are also observed.60 Difficulties in near vision due to accommodative issues and/or uncorrected refractive errors may also contribute to oculomotor dysfunction, as suggested by Liu et al.61 Therefore, a comprehensive visual examination that includes binocular, accommodative, and sensory assessments is necessary to rule out the presence of amblyopia, strabismus, non-strabismic binocular dysfunctions, and oculomotor dysfunctions. Such an approach would reduce the number of visual variables that interfere with measurement and facilitate the establishment of normative oculomotor parameters. It is also essential to measure monocular and binocular visual acuity at both distance and near. Furthermore, it should be noted that the presence of strabismus can result in a number of abnormalities across numerous brain areas involved in visual functions and eye movements.62,63 Indeed, not all eye tracker software is designed for the precise measurement of strabismus. Some software merely detects the initial position of the eyes during calibration, without the capability to distinguish whether the eyes are aligned or if strabismus is present. Considering that oculomotor function is altered when patients do not use the best optical correction or have other visual anomalies,64 the low rate of studies including an optometric evaluation in their sample represents an important limitation (2024 Cacho-Martínez). Other potential biases and applicability concerns have been analyzed in this paper using the QUADAS-2 tool (Table 3, Fig. 2).

Regarding the methods used to measure oculomotor function in the articles included in this systematic review, Table 1 highlights the presence of various stimuli, measurement distances, technologies, and types of visual tasks. Starting with the stimuli employed, a wide range of tests was used, including classic tests such as DEM, NSUCO, K-D, or reading a text, as well as the use of computers to project points, shapes, letters, or cartoons, and even a video game or the OculoMotor Assessment Tool (OMAT). This variety of stimuli, differing in shape, size, color, and cognitive processing difficulty, represents a limitation when attempting to compare them. In fact, the cognitive processes required to follow a moving point differ significantly from those involved in reading text, numbers, or shapes, and are distinct from observing drawings or playing a video game.12 Therefore, this variability constitutes a critical factor when measuring oculomotor function.

During the measurement process, attention loss due to the appearance of another stimulus or, in the case of computers or screens, the presence of additional objects can influence the results. Similarly, the working distance is another limitation. As shown in Table 1, four studies (10.5 %) performed measurements at a near distance (≤ 40 cm),14,18,28,52 18 studies (47.4 %) at an intermediate distance (between 40 and 100 cm),22,24,25,27,29,31,35,38,40,41,43,45–48,51,53,54 seven studies (18.4 %) at a medium-far distance (≥100 cm),19,21,24,34,36,37,39 and nine studies (23.7 %) did not specify the working distance.15,23,30,32,33,42,44,49,50 Although no gold standard exists for the working distance, it can affect the accuracy of eye movement recordings. While it is true that the eye's angular orientation relative to the stimulus is key—and at different distances but the same angular orientation, the eyes should theoretically behave similarly in their movement—factors such as peripheral vision capacity or the limitation of specific ocular muscles may affect the measurement. At greater distances, these factors could introduce errors. These errors are due to the interaction between the accommodative and vergence system,65,66 and it has been shown in scientific literature that variation in working distance produces variation in the accommodative and vergence system.67

Regarding the technology used, there has been significant progress from the early methods, such as electrooculography (an invasive technique), to the development of video nystagmography, video oculography, and the most widely used technology: eye trackers. The use of eye trackers in studies has grown exponentially, particularly in the optometric field, enabling more precise and objective measurements of oculomotor function. However, it is essential to consider that each technology differs in its stimuli, working distances, and limitations, complicating comparisons between studies due to the lack of a gold standard for measurement.

Other factors that may influence results include the ergonomics of the measurement systems. Modern systems, such as eye trackers or video-oculographs, exhibit considerable variability, including trial frames with side-mounted cameras, helmets, virtual reality glasses, and bars placed on computer screens. Additionally, some devices are not commercially available but are laboratory prototypes or optical bench setups. Establishing unified ergonomic criteria would be beneficial.

During the evaluation of oculomotor function, it is essential to consider the subject's age. Scientific literature has demonstrated that oculomotor function varies with age, and studies that propose normative values present results based on age.19,47–49,52–54 Research suggests that oculomotor performance improves during the early years of life (approximately from 4 to 15 years)68–70 and subsequently declines.55,70,71 Another critical aspect to consider is the subject's cognitive level. Neurodevelopmental disorders,72,73 neurological diseases,74,75 or, as mentioned earlier, age—particularly in early childhood and older adulthood—can significantly influence the measurements.

Furthermore, maintaining the subject's attention is crucial. Oculomotor measurements typically last between 30 s and 5 min, and any loss of attention during the evaluation can lead to inconsistent or unreliable results. Similarly, fatigue and exhaustion during the test—especially in lengthy, tedious evaluations involving complex stimuli or requiring high cognitive demand—can introduce biases into the measurements.76 A clear parallel can be observed in visual field testing, where efforts have been made for years to shorten the test duration to mitigate these biases.

All these factors—age, cognitive level, attention, fatigue, and exhaustion—must be carefully considered when establishing a gold standard for oculomotor function measurement. Unified criteria should be defined to ensure consistency and reliability in the evaluation process.

Following the identification of methodological considerations frequently neglected in the literature and warranting careful attention, it is equally imperative to undertake a critical appraisal of the overall quality and methodological rigor of the included studies. In this systematic review, this analysis was conducted using the QUADAS-2 tool (Table 3, Fig. 2), which demonstrated a high incidence of studies with significant risks of bias and important limitations regarding applicability concerns. In particular, the most common risk of bias is related to reference standards, as only 4 out of 38 studies (10.5 %)28,33,48,53 compare the test under study with a gold standard technique or at least other independent method capable of measuring similar parameters of oculomotor function. This lack of comparison prevents validation of the measurements due to the impossibility of performing an agreement analysis. Similarly, flow and timing are also highly affected by a high risk of bias in almost all studies, with only 3 out of 38 studies (7.9 %)14,15,21 being free of this type of bias. The bias was generally caused by the predominance of single measurements per device, which prevents repeatability analysis. This finding further contributes to the generation of non-validated measurements that cannot be used as normative values.

Patient selection is another significant source of bias present in most studies, as many included subjects with highly specific characteristics–for example, only highly educated individuals40 or only students50–limiting extrapolation to the general population. Additionally, the selection of normal subjects was often influenced by the demographics and characteristics of other study groups involved in comparative studies. Therefore, recruitment in these comparative studies is not randomized. Furthermore, comparative studies often attempt to match ages between groups, leading to significant heterogeneity among control groups depending on the nature of the study.

On the other hand, it is important to highlight the higher-quality results regarding measurement methodology or index test in the current review, as 27 out of 38 studies (71.1 %) provide a complete description of the procedure.14,18,19,22–24,27,29,32–39,42–49,52–54 However, despite an accurate description of the measurement process, technology, and tasks, in some cases, the methodology remains difficult to replicate, affecting its applicability. This limitation is primary due to the use of outdated technology,21–23 implementation of subjective tests,15 non-standardized texts that introduce cognitive and educational dependencies,40,54 and highly complex tests that are difficult to replicate, such as the Convirt Test50 or OMAT Test.52

Applicability concerns are further compounded by unclear or questionable inclusion criteria for subjects, as well as the lack of proper characterization of measurements, leading to data that do not contribute to establishing normative values.

Regarding the limitations of the present study, the scarcity of specific articles evaluating normative values must be noted, as most studies focus on comparative or interventional approaches rather than the characterization of oculomotor function. Additionally, intrinsic limitations of the current tools used to assess study quality may lead to incomplete considerations or misinterpretations, even though QUADAS-2 remains the most appropriate tool for the objectives of this investigation. Because of these limitations a unique standardization of oculomotor measurements is currently not possible. Future studies should be conducted to propose a preliminary oculomotor protocol by an expert committee which should be validated in different sample characteristics.

On the other hand, key strengths of this study include its innovative and applicable focus, emphasizing the need to establish normative values. Moreover, the development and analysis of the procedure were conducted independently by three investigators, with a final consensus to enhance the evaluation of the current evidence.

Conclusion

Based on these findings, this systematic review concludes that there is no clear evidence on normative values for oculomotor skills, nor is there a consensus on the measurement methods, stimuli used, or working distances. Furthermore, there is no agreement on which specific aspects of oculomotor skills should be assessed, and due to the quality evaluation of these studies, the risk of bias remains predominantly high.

Additionally, it is concerning that the vast majority of studies do not include a comprehensive optometric assessment – incorporating refractive, accommodative, and binocular tests – which may introduce bias in the results. To enhance reliability and applicability, measurement criteria should be standardized, and normative values should be established.

Declaration of competing interest

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

Funding

No funding.

References
[1]
S.M. Doettl, D.L. McCaslin.
Oculomotor assessment in children.
Semin Hear, 39 (2018), pp. 275-287
[2]
A. Salgado-Fernandez, A. Vazquez-Amor, C. Alvarez-Peregrin, C. Martinez-Perez, C. Villa-Collar, M. Angel Sanchez-Tena.
Influence of eye movements on academic performance: a bibliometric and citation network analysis.
J Eye Mov Res, 15 (2022),
[3]
A. Wolf, K. Ueda, Y. Hirano.
Recent updates of eye movement abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia: a scoping review.
Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 75 (2021), pp. 82-100
[4]
A. Wolf, K. Tripanpitak, S. Umeda, M. Otake-Matsuura.
Eye-tracking paradigms for the assessment of mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review.
Front Psychol, 14 (2023),
[5]
B. Noyes, A. Biorac, G. Vazquez, S. Khalid-Khan, D. Munoz, L. Booij.
Eye-tracking in adult depression: protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis.
BMJ Open, 13 (2023),
[6]
A. Sekar, M.T.N. Panouilleres, D. Kaski.
Detecting abnormal eye movements in patients with neurodegenerative diseases: current insights.
Eye Brain, 16 (2024), pp. 3-16
[7]
C. Bilbao, D.P. Pinero.
Clinical characterization of oculomotricity in children with and without specific learning disorders.
Brain Sci, 10 (2020),
[8]
V. Vinuela-Navarro, J. Goset, M. Aldaba, et al.
Eye movements in patients with post-COVID condition.
Biomed Opt Express, 14 (2023), pp. 3936-3949
[9]
C.M. Gheorghe, V.L. Purcarea, I.R. Gheorghe.
Using eye-tracking technology in Neuromarketing.
Rom J Ophthalmol, 67 (2023), pp. 2-6
[10]
R. Spataro, M. Ciriacono, C. Manno, V. La Bella.
The eye-tracking computer device for communication in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis.
Acta Neurol Scand, 130 (2014), pp. 40-45
[11]
L. Gonzalez-Vides, J.L. Hernandez-Verdejo, P. Canadas-Suarez.
Eye tracking in optometry: a systematic review.
J Eye Mov Res, 16 (2023),
[12]
W. Nieboer, A. Ghiani, R. de Vries, E. Brenner, D.L. Mann.
Eye tracking to assess the functional consequences of vision impairment: a systematic review.
Optom Vis Sci, 100 (2023), pp. 861-875
[13]
D.P. Pinero.
Oculomotor dysfunctions: evidence-based practice.
J Optom, 13 (2020), pp. 137-138
[14]
R.P. Garzia, J.E. Richman, S.B. Nicholson, C.S. Gaines.
A new visual-verbal saccade test: the development eye movement test (DEM).
J Am Optom Assoc, 61 (1990), pp. 124-135
[15]
W.C. Maples, J. Atchley, T. Ficklin.
Northeastern State University College of Optometry’s oculomotor norms.
J Behav Optom, 3 (1992), pp. 143-150
[16]
S. Lieberman, A.H. Cohen, J. Rubin.
NYSOA K-D test.
J Am Optom Assoc, 54 (1983), pp. 631-637
[17]
I.S. Curthoys, S. Ooi, S.L. Wearne, M.J. Todd, G.M. Halmagyi.
Normative data for human smooth pursuit responses to horizontal step-ramp targets.
Acta Otolaryngol Suppl, 481 (1991), pp. 391-393
[18]
A. Rutsche, A. Baumann, X. Jiang, D.S. Mojon.
Development of visual pursuit in the first 6 years of life.
Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol, 244 (2006), pp. 1406-1411
[19]
S. Sinno, F. Najem, K.S. Abouchacra, P. Perrin, G. Dumas.
Normative values of saccades and smooth pursuit in children aged 5 to 17 years.
J Am Acad Audiol, 31 (2020), pp. 384-392
[20]
S.J. Wilson, P. Glue, D. Ball, D.J. Nutt.
Saccadic eye movement parameters in normal subjects.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 86 (1993), pp. 69-74
[21]
L. Schalen.
Quantification of tracking eye movements in normal subjects.
Acta Otolaryngol, 90 (1980), pp. 404-413
[22]
J. Bergenius.
Computerized analysis of voluntary eye movements. A clinical method for evaluation of smooth pursuit and saccades in oto-neurological diagnosis.
Acta Otolaryngol, 98 (1984), pp. 490-500
[23]
M. Versino, M. Grassi, E. Genovese, D. Zambarbieri, R. Schmid, V. Cosi.
Quantitative evaluation of saccadic eye movements.
Neuro-Ophthalmol, 12 (1992), pp. 327-342
[24]
R.G. Ross, A.D. Radant, D.W. Hommer.
A developmental study of smooth pursuit eye movements in normal children from 7 to 15 years of age.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry, 32 (1993), pp. 783-791
[25]
R.E. Litman, E.F. Torrey, D.W. Hommer, A.R. Radant, D. Pickar, D.R. Weinberger.
A quantitative analysis of smooth pursuit eye tracking in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry, 54 (1997), pp. 417-426
[26]
D.E. Ross, G.K. Thaker, R.W. Buchanan, et al.
Eye tracking disorder in schizophrenia is characterized by specific ocular motor defects and is associated with the deficit syndrome.
Biol Psychiatry, 42 (1997), pp. 781-796
[27]
A. Campana, A. Duci, O. Gambini, S. Scarone.
An artificial neural network that uses eye-tracking performance to identify patients with schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull, 25 (1999), pp. 789-799
[28]
R. Jimenez, M.D. Gonzalez, M.A. Perez, J.A. Garcia.
Evolution of accommodative function and development of ocular movements in children.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt, 23 (2003), pp. 97-107
[29]
E. Aring, M.A. Gronlund, A. Hellstrom, J. Ygge.
Visual fixation development in children.
Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol, 245 (2007), pp. 1659-1665
[30]
M.J. van Tricht, D.H. Nieman, L.J. Bour, et al.
Increased saccadic rate during smooth pursuit eye movements in patients at ultra high risk for developing a psychosis.
Brain Cogn, 73 (2010), pp. 215-221
[31]
J. Goepel, S.C. Biehl, J. Kissler, I. Paul-Jordanov.
Pro- and antisaccades in children elicited by visual and acoustic targets - does modality matter?.
BMC Pediatr, 11 (2011), pp. 116
[32]
E. Kattoulas, N. Smyrnis, N.C. Stefanis, D. Avramopoulos, C.N. Stefanis, I. Evdokimidis.
Predictive smooth eye pursuit in a population of young men: I. Effects of age, IQ, oculomotor and cognitive tasks.
Exp Brain Res, 215 (2011), pp. 207-218
[33]
A. Webber, J. Wood, G. Gole, B. Brown.
DEM test, visagraph eye movement recordings, and reading ability in children.
Optom Vis Sci, 88 (2011), pp. 295-302
[34]
X.F. Shi, L.M. Xu, Y. Li, T. Wang, K.X. Zhao, B.A. Sabel.
Fixational saccadic eye movements are altered in anisometropic amblyopia.
Restor Neurol Neurosci, 30 (2012), pp. 445-462
[35]
F.H. Boot, J.J. Pel, H.M. Evenhuis, J. van der Steen.
Quantification of visual orienting responses to coherent form and motion in typically developing children aged 0-12 years.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci, 53 (2012), pp. 2708-2714
[36]
F. Seferlis, T.S. Chimona, C.E. Papadakis, J. Bizakis, S. Triaridis, C. Skoulakis.
Age related changes in ocular motor testing in healthy subjects.
J Vestib Res, 25 (2015), pp. 57-66
[37]
S.M. Doettl, P.N. Plyler, D.L. McCaslin, N.L. Schay.
Pediatric oculomotor findings during monocular videonystagmography: a developmental study.
J Am Acad Audiol, 26 (2015), pp. 703-715
[38]
T. Zalla, M. Seassau, F. Cazalis, D. Gras, M. Leboyer.
Saccadic eye movements in adults with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder.
Autism, 22 (2018), pp. 195-204
[39]
C.J. Yang, J.Y. Lee, B.C. Kang, H.S. Lee, M.H. Yoo, H.J. Park.
Quantitative analysis of gains and catch-up saccades of video-head-impulse testing by age in normal subjects.
Clin Otolaryngol, 41 (2016), pp. 532-538
[40]
W. Choi, M.W. Lowder, F. Ferreira, T.Y. Swaab, J.M. Henderson.
Effects of word predictability and preview lexicality on eye movements during reading: a comparison between young and older adults.
Psychol Aging, 32 (2017), pp. 232-242
[41]
M. Ferreira, P.A. Pereira, M. Parreira, et al.
Using endogenous saccades to characterize fatigue in multiple sclerosis.
Mult Scler Relat Disord, 14 (2017), pp. 16-22
[42]
J.R. Rizzo, T.E. Hudson, P.X. Amorapanth, et al.
The effect of linguistic background on rapid number naming: implications for native versus non-native english speakers on sideline-focused concussion assessments.
Brain Inj, 32 (2018), pp. 1690-1699
[43]
P.A. Wetzel, A.S. Lindblad, H. Raizada, et al.
Eye tracking results in postconcussive syndrome versus normative participants.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci, 59 (2018), pp. 4011-4019
[44]
A. Hoffmann, U. Ettinger, C. Montoro, G.A. Reyes Del Paso, S. Duschek.
Cerebral blood flow responses during prosaccade and antisaccade preparation in major depression.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 269 (2019), pp. 813-822
[45]
N. Chehrehnegar, V. Nejati, M. Shati, et al.
Behavioral and cognitive markers of mild cognitive impairment: diagnostic value of saccadic eye movements and Simon task.
Aging Clin Exp Res, 31 (2019), pp. 1591-1600
[46]
P.A. Wetzel, A.S. Lindblad, C. Mulatya, et al.
Eye tracker outcomes in a randomized trial of 40 sessions of hyperbaric oxygen or sham in participants with persistent post concussive symptoms.
Undersea Hyperb Med, 46 (2019), pp. 299-311
[47]
J. Wertli, A. Schotzau, S. Trauzettel-Klosinski, A. Palmowski-Wolfe.
Feasibility of eye movement recordings with the SMI tracking bar in 10- to 11-year-old children performing a reading task.
Klin Monbl Augenheilkd, 237 (2020), pp. 510-516
[48]
G.P. Hindmarsh, A.A. Black, S.L. White, S. Hopkins, J.M. Wood.
Eye movement patterns and reading ability in children.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt, 41 (2021), pp. 1134-1143
[49]
A. Kullmann, R.C. Ashmore, A. Braverman, et al.
Portable eye-tracking as a reliable assessment of oculomotor, cognitive and reaction time function: normative data for 18-45 year old.
PLoS One, 16 (2021),
[50]
I. Amato, A. Nanev, S. Piantella, et al.
Assessing the utility of a virtual-reality neuropsychological test battery, 'CONVIRT', in detecting alcohol-induced cognitive impairment.
Behav Res Methods, 53 (2021), pp. 1115-1123
[51]
F. D'Addio, I. Pastore, C. Loretelli, et al.
Abnormalities of the oculomotor function in type 1 diabetes and diabetic neuropathy.
Acta Diabetol, 59 (2022), pp. 1157-1167
[52]
R. Eichler, G. Mivtachi, D. Hershkovitz-Azoulay, M. Scheiman, H. Ben-Eli.
OculoMotor assessment tool: children compared with adults.
Clin Optom (Auckl), 14 (2022), pp. 75-81
[53]
E. Orduna-Hospital, A. Navarro-Marques, C. Lopez-de-la-Fuente, A. Sanchez-Cano.
Eye-tracker study of the developmental Eye movement test in young people without binocular dysfunctions.
Life (Basel), 13 (2023), pp. 773
[54]
J. Wertli, A. Schotzau, A. Palmowski-Wolfe.
The influence of age on eye movements during reading in early elementary school children.
Klin Monbl Augenheilkd, 240 (2023), pp. 591-598
[55]
V. Pueyo, J.C.S. Yam, T. Perez-Roche, et al.
Development of oculomotor control throughout childhood: a multicenter and multiethnic study.
[56]
A.L. Webber.
The functional impact of amblyopia.
Clin Exp Optom, 101 (2018), pp. 443-450
[57]
M. Hepokur, B. Mutlu, M. Gunes, et al.
The effect of different types of convergent strabismus on horizontal eye movements.
Int Ophthalmol, 42 (2022), pp. 3951-3961
[58]
E.E. Birch, K.R. Kelly, D.E. Giaschi.
Fellow eye deficits in amblyopia.
J Binocul Vis Ocul Motil, 69 (2019), pp. 116-125
[59]
J. Otero-Millan, S.L. Macknik, S. Martinez-Conde.
Fixational eye movements and binocular vision.
Front Integr Neurosci, 8 (2014), pp. 52
[60]
M. Hirota, H. Kanda, T. Endo, et al.
Relationship between reading performance and saccadic disconjugacy in patients with convergence insufficiency type intermittent exotropia.
Jpn J Ophthalmol, 60 (2016), pp. 326-332
[61]
Z. Liu, K. Zhang, S. Gao, J. Yang, W. Qiu.
Correlation between eye movements and asthenopia: a prospective observational study.
J Clin Med, 11 (2022), pp. 7043
[62]
H.S. Metz.
Restrictive factors in strabismus.
Surv Ophthalmol, 28 (1983), pp. 71-83
[63]
M.M.G. Walton, A. Pallus, J. Fleuriet, M.J. Mustari, K. Tarczy-Hornoch.
Neural mechanisms of oculomotor abnormalities in the infantile strabismus syndrome.
J Neurophysiol, 118 (2017), pp. 280-299
[64]
M. Scheiman, B. Wick.
Clinical Management of Binocular Vision. Heterophoric, Accommodative, and Eye Movement Disorders, Wolters Kluwer, (2019),
[65]
G.K. Hung.
Adaptation model of accommodation and vergence.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt, 12 (1992), pp. 319-326
[66]
G.K. Hung, K.J. Ciuffreda, M. Rosenfield.
Proximal contribution to a linear static model of accommodation and vergence.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt, 16 (1996), pp. 31-41
[67]
M. Rosenfield.
Tonic vergence and vergence adaptation.
Optom Vis Sci, 74 (1997), pp. 303-328
[68]
A.P. Accardo, S. Pensiero, S. Da Pozzo, P. Perissutti.
Characteristics of horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements to sinusoidal stimulation in children of primary school age.
Vision Res, 35 (1995), pp. 539-548
[69]
D. Purves, G.J. Augustine, D. Fitzpatrick, et al.
Types of eye movements and their functions.
Neuroscience, Sinauer Associates, (2001),
[70]
B. Luna, K. Velanova, C.F. Geier.
Development of eye-movement control.
Brain Cogn, 68 (2008), pp. 293-308
[71]
P. Blignaut, E.J. van Rensburg, M. Oberholzer.
Visualization and quantification of eye tracking data for the evaluation of oculomotor function.
Heliyon, 5 (2019),
[72]
C. Bilbao, D.P. Pinero.
Objective and subjective evaluation of saccadic eye movements in healthy children and children with neurodevelopmental disorders: a pilot study.
Vision (Basel), 5 (2021), pp. 28
[73]
C. Bilbao, D.P. Pinero.
Distribution of visual and oculomotor alterations in a clinical population of children with and without neurodevelopmental disorders.
Brain Sci, 11 (2021), pp. 351
[74]
S. Caldani, I. Amado, N. Bendjemaa, et al.
Oculomotricity and neurological soft signs: can we refine the endophenotype? A study in subjects belonging to the spectrum of schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res, 256 (2017), pp. 490-497
[75]
A. Gil-Casas, D.P. Pinero, A. Molina-Martin.
Are near visual signs and symptoms in multiple sclerosis compatible with convergence insufficiency?.
Clin Exp Optom, 105 (2022), pp. 631-636
[76]
B.B. Bruce, N.J. Newman.
Functional visual loss.
Neurol Clin, 28 (2010), pp. 789-802
Copyright © 2025. The Authors
Download PDF
Journal of Optometry
Article options
Tools
Supplemental materials

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?