Journal Information
Vol. 15. Issue 2.
Pages 186 (April - June 2022)
Share
Share
Download PDF
More article options
Vol. 15. Issue 2.
Pages 186 (April - June 2022)
Letters to the editor
Open Access
Mallett unit or fully fusionable images for prisms against asthenopia?
Visits
2438
Guntram Kommerell
Corresponding author
Guntram.kommerell@t-online.de

Corresponding author.
, Michael Bach
Eye Center, Faculty of Medicine, Medical Center – University of Freiburg, University of Freiburg, Germany
Related content
Bruce John William Evans, Ketan Parmar, Christine Dickinson
This item has received

Under a Creative Commons license
Article information
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Full Text

Parmar et al.1 remarked that a “dissociative environment” could produce anomalous results of Mallett's test. They cite work from our lab,2 suggesting that our use of rotary Risley prisms could have brought about such a dissociative environment. Yes, the frame of the Risley prisms restricted the peripheral visual field, but this appeared acceptable, since the text of the Mallett unit around the target remained fully visible. We had a good reason to apply the continuously adjustable Risley prisms rather than putting on prisms step by step: Risley prisms allowed our subjects to choose the appropriate power on their own, thus avoiding suggestive influences from the examiner, and to record the procedure with a computer.

Sensibly, Mallett aimed at viewing conditions as normal as possible in order to find the smallest prism that could alleviate asthenopia. In his test, the only deviations from normal vision are monocular markers in the form of Nonius lines. To examine whether these monocular markers represent a source of artefact, we determined the vergence position of rest (the position in which the sensory-motor system is unburdened from any strain to fuse the images of the two eyes), comparing Mallett's display with and without the Nonius lines. With the monocular markers, our subjects were asked to align the Nonius lines. Without the monocular markers, i.e., with fully fusionable pictures, our subjects adjusted the prism so that viewing appeared most relaxing. We found that the vergence position of rest differed up to 7 prism diopters between the two conditions, with an overall correlation of only r ≈ 0.75. This suggests that monocular markers lead to an artefact, possibly via binocular rivalry in the area of the Nonius lines.

To determine the influence of the monocular markers, we deliberately aimed at the full vergence position of rest, not at the smallest power of the prism that aligns the Nonius lines. The latter strategy would have been appropriate for the prescription of therapeutic prisms, according to Mallett's recommendations.

A large body of research supports the use of the Mallett unit for finding prisms that can alleviate asthenopic symptoms. Our study suggests that it may be worthwhile to investigate whether even better results can be obtained by presenting natural stimuli without monocular markers, relying on the patient's sense of comfort. Many of Mallett's recommendations may apply to this approach.

References
[1]
K.R. Parmar, C. Dickinson, B.J. Evans.
Does an iPad fixation disparity test give equivalent results to the Mallett near fixation disparity test?.
J Optom, 12 (2019), pp. 222-231
[2]
G. Kommerell, M. Kromeier, F. Scharff, et al.
Asthenopia, associated phoria, and self-selected prism.
Strabismus, 23 (2015), pp. 51-65
Copyright © 2021. Spanish General Council of Optometry
Journal of Optometry
Article options
Tools

Are you a health professional able to prescribe or dispense drugs?